By: Richard D. Arvey
Industrial Relations Center, University of
Minnesota Thomas J. Bouchard
Jr. University of Minnesota
Nancy L. Segal University
of Minnesota Lauren M. Abraham
Industrial Relations Center, University of
Minnesota
Acknowledgement:
This research was partially supported by grants
to the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart from the
Pioneer Fund, The Seaver Institute, the Koch Charitable
Foundation, the Spencer Foundation, Harcourt Brace
Jovanovich, the National Science Foundation
(BNS-7026654), and the Graduate School and the
Industrial Relations Center of the University of
Minnesota. We wish to express appreciation to Matt McGue
and the anonymous reviewers for their comments on
earlier versions of this article.
Correspondence concerning this
article should be addressed to: Richard D. Arvey,
Industrial Relations Center, University of Minnesota,
Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455
Personal experience informs us
that some coworkers or friends seem dissatisfied across
a variety of job circumstances, whereas other
individuals appear satisfied regardless of past and
present job histories. This observation is buttressed by
the work of Staw
and Ross (1985), who examined the consistency of
cross-situational job satisfaction in a national random
sample of over 5,000 men. Their data revealed a
correlation of .33 between a one-item measure of job
satisfaction that was taken in 1969 and 1971 for
individuals who experienced both occupational and
employer changes. On the basis of these and other data,
Staw and Ross argued that dispositional influences
importantly influence job attitudes and that perhaps too
much attention had been given to specific environmental
aspects of jobs as determinants of job attitudes. That
is, stable individual differences might be as important
a factor in determining job attitudes as that of the job
or work environment itself. A recent study by Gerhart
(1987) underscored the notion that both
dispositional and situational factors are associated
with job satisfaction. He found evidence for consistency
in job satisfaction across employer and occupational
changes using a youth cohort. Moreover, when jobs were
coded for complexity and the impact of changes in job
complexity on job satisfaction were examined, changes in
job satisfaction were found to be significantly
associated with changes in job complexity.
In their provocative article, Staw
and Ross (1985) articulated several individual
difference characteristics that might underlie the
dispositional explanation of job attitudes. They
suggested, for example, that:
Job attitudes may reflect a biologically based
trait that predisposes individuals to see positive or
negative content in their lives. … Differences in
individual temperament, … ranging from clinical
depression to a very positive disposition, could
influence the information individuals input, recall,
and interpret within various social situations,
including work. (p. 471)
Staw,
Bell, and Clausen (1986) conducted further
investigation of the impact of dispositions on job
attitudes. They found consistent evidence of affective
dispositions influencing job attitudes over long periods
of time. Although they could document the stability of
dispositions over time, they asserted that it was
impossible to determine if affective dispositions
originated from genetic or social forces and how
external factors might influence these affective
states.
It seems reasonable that genetic
factors might influence the manner in which individuals
respond to their work contexts. Genetic influences on a
wide range of individual differences have been well
documented (Rowe,
1987). Examples include general intelligence (Bouchard
& McGue, 1981; Teasdale
& Owen, 1984), information processing (McGue
& Bouchard, in press), personality dispositions
(Bouchard,
1984; Goldsmith,
1983; Tellegen
et al., 1988), psychological interests (Nichols,
1978), and attitudes (Martin
et al., 1986). No study, however, has directly
examined the degree to which genetic factors might
influence job attitudes. We address this question in
this study.
Job satisfaction is commonly
viewed as multidimensional (Locke,
1976), raising the possibility that particular
elements of job satisfaction vary with respect to the
degree of influence of genetic factors. We suggest that
job satisfaction dimensions or elements that explicitly
represent extrinsic work environmental factors (e.g.,
the working conditions, supervision, etc.) are less
likely to demonstrate genetic components than job
satisfaction elements that may reflect more direct
experiences of the job by individuals (i.e., the
“intrinsic” aspects of job satisfaction, such as
challenge or achievement). The distinction between these
two generic types of job satisfaction elements has been
described in earlier research (Muchinsky,
1983; Wernimont,
1966). Thus, we offer a preliminary prediction that
genetic influence will be stronger for intrinsic job
satisfaction indicators than for extrinsic job
satisfaction variables. Method
This study involved the
measurement of job attitudes of monozygotic twins who
were reared apart (MZA) from an early age. The use of
twins for determining the relative contribution of
genetic and environmental factors in explaining the
variance of any reliably measured variable is well known
(Hay,
1985; Rowe,
1987; Willerman,
1979). Monozygotic twins reared together (MZT) share
the same genetic structure and common family
environment. Any similarity between such twins may,
therefore, be determined by one or both of these two
broad classes of influences. Dizygotic twins reared
together (DZT) share one-half of their genes (on
average) in common by descent. This method of estimating
the contribution of genetic factors assumes equality of
environmental variance for both types of twins. Given
this assumption, twice the difference between the two
types of twins estimates the magnitude of genetic
influences. The monozygotic twin reared apart (MZA)
design is much simpler. In the absence of selective
placement (we assume random placement with respect to
trait-relevant environments) the intraclass correlation
between such pairs is a direct estimate of the genetic
contribution (broad-sense heritability) to any measured
variable (Bouchard,
1984, 1987;
Plomin,
DeFries, & McClearn, 1980). MZA twins are
relatively rare. The study of such twins offers a unique
opportunity to determine the magnitude of genetic
influences on traits of interest.
Sample
The MZA twins in this study
participated in the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared
Apart between 1979 and 1987. Details of their
recruitment were reported in Bouchard
(1984, 1987)
and the references cited therein. The zygosity diagnosis
of all twins included in this study was based on
analyses of eight blood group systems, four serum
proteins, six red blood cell enzymes, fingerprint
ridgecount, ponderal index, and cephalic index.
Probability of misdiagnosis is less than 0.001 (Lykken,
1978). From 1983 to 1987, all twin pairs who
participated in the study were administered a work
history questionnaire containing job satisfaction items
as part of the assessment battery. The same work history
questionnaire was also mailed to twin pairs who had
previously participated in the study but who had not
completed the job satisfaction items. Thirty-four
monozygotic twin pairs provided complete data. The mean
age of these individuals was 41.88 years (SD =
12.03). This sample included 25 female twin pairs and 9
male twin pairs. The mean age of separation for the
sample was .45 years (SD = .79), indicating
that the twin pairs were separated quite early in their
lives. The mean age of reunion was 31.71 years
(SD = 15.77). Almost two-thirds of the
participants were engaged in their major job at the time
of testing, and 62% reported well-above-average success
in their jobs.
Measures
We asked respondents to complete
the short form of the Minnesota Job Satisfaction
Questionnaire (MSQ) developed by Weiss,
Dawis, England, and Lofquist (1967). They were asked
to respond to questions concerning the job that they had
held for the longest period of their lives or the job
that they considered to be their “major job.” The
category housewife was considered as a major
job and respondents were asked to respond as such (Arvey
& Gross, 1977). The number and types of jobs
that respondents targeted as their major job were wide
and diverse, ranging from research chemist to coal
miner. Of the 68 MZ twins, 17 indicated that they were
responding to housewife as their major work role.
The MSQ is a well-regarded
measure of job satisfaction (Muchinsky,
1983) and has been used in numerous studies. The
short form of the inventory includes 20 items that are
relevant to a number of job facets and on which
respondents indicate their degree of relative
satisfaction using a 5-point scale (1 = very
dissatisfied, 5 = very satisfied). The
manual for the MSQ indicates that factor analytic
procedures have produced an “intrinsic” satisfaction
scale and an “extrinsic” satisfaction scale. The
intrinsic satisfaction scale consists of those 12 items
that reflect ability utilization, achievement, the
chance to do things for other people in the job, and so
forth. The extrinsic subscale consists of 6 items that
concern the way company policies are administered, the
quality of working conditions, and so forth. The general
satisfaction subscale is simply a summation of the 20
items and can be viewed as a composite of all the facets
of job satisfaction. The manual indicates that the
internal consistency measures calculated for these
scales, on the basis of a wide variety of occupational
groups, produces a median reliability coefficient of .86
for the intrinsic satisfaction scale, .80 for the
extrinsic scale, and .90 for the general satisfaction
scale.
We formed scales for extrinsic,
intrinsic, and general satisfaction for each respondent
using the MSQ manual as our guide to which of the items
were scored on each scale. We compared the means and
standard deviations of the intrinsic, extrinsic, and
general satisfaction scales in the sample with those
presented in the manual to determine if the sample
differed substantially from other normative groups. The
MSQ manual reports the means for these three scales as
47.14, 19.98, and 74.85, respectively, on the basis of a
sample of 1,723 individuals representing a variety of
occupational areas. The respective values obtained in
the present sample were 49.37, 21.23, and 78.64. The
standard deviations were 7.42, 4.78, and 11.92 for the
normative group, and 6.73, 4.56, and 10.78 for the
present sample. Compared with the normative group, the
sample of MZA twins expressed slightly higher
satisfaction on each of the three scales; although these
differences were statistically significant for all three
scales, the magnitude of the differences was not
substantial. The differences between the standard
deviations for the sample and the normative group were
quite modest, further suggesting that the twin sample
was quite similar to the larger population sample.
In addition to the satisfaction
items and subscales, a single item designed to measure
overall job satisfaction was included in the
questionnaire. This measure was also scored on a 5-point
scale (1 = very dissatisfied and 5 = very
satisfied).
Finally, each job was assigned
several scores derived from the 4th edition of the
Dictionary of Occupational Titles (DOT; U.S.
Department of Labor, 1977) by Roos
& Treiman (1980). These scores have been used by
Gerhart
(1987) in developing relatively objective
non-self-report-based indices of job characteristics.
Using the method described by Gerhart
(1987) and Roos
& Treiman (1980), jobs are assigned scores on
the relative complexity, motor skills, physical demands,
and undesirable working conditions associated with their
respective tasks, duties, and responsibilities. This
scoring system represents average levels of complexity,
motor skill demands, and so forth for entire occupations
rather than for specific jobs. Thus, there is
considerable room for departure from these values
because of interoccupational job differences that would
introduce error into this variable in addition to rater
error. The scoring system is factorially derived, so
that scores on the complexity variable reflect DOT items
such as functional complexity in relation to data,
required intelligence, and required abstract reasoning,
whereas scores on the physical demands variable reflect
items involving such features as eye–hand coordination,
climbing, hazards, and so forth.
In the present study, each job
was coded independently by two raters who were blind to
the twin pairing. Jobs that were based on a full sample
of individuals who completed a work-history
questionnaire as part of a larger study (monozygotic
twins, dizygotic twins and their spouses, N =
146) were coded. The correlations between the two raters
for the complexity, motor skills, physical demands, and
working conditions scales across all jobs were .85, .69,
.77, and .70, respectively. When the cases representing
the housewives category were removed from these
analyses, similar levels of interrater agreement were
obtained. These values are consistent with the
reliabilities reported by Gerhart
(1987). Scores were subsequently obtained for each
of the four scales by averaging across the two raters.
We report the means and standard deviations for the
satisfaction and DOT-based job ratings in Table
1.

Procedure
The data were analyzed in two
phases. In Phase 1, all items and subscales were
adjusted for the effects of age and sex (prior to
computing the intraclass correlations) following the
procedures outlined by McGue
and Bouchard (1984). These authors have pointed out
that when the variables of interest are correlated with
age or sex (or with both) and are not adjusted, the twin
correlations represent overestimates of genetic
influence. We used the data based on the full sample set
of monozygotic and dizygotic twins (N = 146) to
determine the correlation of each of the MSQ items with
age and sex. This information was used to correct the
variables in terms of age and sex. We then computed
F tests to determine if the coefficients were
significantly different from zero. To determine if the
intraclass correlation for the intrinsic satisfaction
subscale differed from that of the extrinsic
satisfaction composite, we performed a one-tailed test
for the equality of two dependent correlations (Snedecor
& Cochran, 1967, p. 295; Steiger,
1980).
Phase 2 analyses involved
determining job similarity by calculating intraclass
correlations for the age- and sex-adjusted DOT-based
measures of complexity, motor skills, physical demands,
and working conditions. As we discuss later, these
DOT-based scores were partialed out of the satisfaction
items and subscales to determine if a significant
genetic component for satisfaction remained after
holding the jobs constant via these DOT-based
scores. Results
In Table
2 we report the mean squares between pairs, mean
squares within pairs, and the intraclass correlation
coefficient for the MSQ job satisfaction variables and
composites on the basis of the sex- and age-adjusted
scores. The prediction that general satisfaction would
demonstrate a significant heritability was confirmed by
these data. The intraclass correlation for the adjusted
scores was .309 (p < .05) for general
satisfaction. When this analysis was completed after
removing any pair that included a housewife, the
intraclass correlation was .304 (p < .08)
for general satisfaction on the basis of a sample of 21
pairs. This estimate is obviously close to that observed
using the full data set, but has a reduced significance
value because of the smaller sample. The intraclass
correlation for the single-item overall satisfaction
scale, however, was not statistically significant
(r = .166).

The age- and sex-adjusted
intraclass correlation for the intrinsic satisfaction
scale was also significantly different from zero (.315,
p < .05), whereas the adjusted intraclass
correlation for the extrinsic satisfaction scale was
only .109 and not significantly different from zero. The
prediction that intrinsic satisfaction would show a
stronger heritability than the extrinsic satisfaction
scale was not confirmed (z = 1.04,
ns).
Reviewing the data on an
item-by-item basis, five of the satisfaction items
demonstrated significant intraclass correlations. These
items involved satisfaction with the chance to be
somebody in the community (Item 4), the way my boss
handles people (Item 5), doing things for other people
(Item 9), the freedom to use my own judgment (Item 15),
and the feeling of accomplishment I get from my job
(Item 20). These data confirmed the hypothesis that
there is a significant genetic component to intrinsic
job satisfaction as well as to general job
satisfaction.
Next, we explored the issue
concerning whether there were similarities in the target
jobs held or described by the twins and whether
partialing out features associated with these jobs would
diminish the heritabilities obtained for job
satisfaction. One factor that might have accounted for
the results was that the twin pairs might have
self-selected similar job environments (so that they
experienced similar reinforcers in those environments).
Such events could conceivably have led their
satisfaction levels to coincide. Plomin
and colleagues (1980) described situations in which
individuals are not passive recipients of their
environments but actively seek environments compatible
with their genetic propensities. They called this
process “active” genotype–environment covariance. Scarr
and McCartney (1983) similarly pointed out that this
form of self-seeking behavior or environmental matching
may be genetically influenced.
To explore the issue of active
genotype–environment covariance, we first calculated the
intraclass correlations using the DOT-derived scores as
dependent variables. If there were a genetically based
propensity to seek out similar jobs, the heritabilities
for these scores would be significant. The intraclass
correlations for complexity, motor skills, and physical
demands were .443, .356, and .338, respectively (all
p s = .05). The intraclass correlation obtained
for working conditions was not significant. These
results represent evidence that the twins held jobs that
were similar in terms of their complexity level, motor
skill requirements, and physical demands. They are also
compatible with the hypothesis that there is a genetic
component in terms of the jobs that are sought and held
by individuals.
To assess the question
concerning whether the heritabilities obtained for the
job satisfaction variables were due, in part, to the
propensities of the twins to hold similar jobs, we
partialed out job complexity, motor skills, and physical
demand scores from the various job satisfaction measures
and recalculated the intraclass correlations for
satisfaction. Only slight changes in the intraclass
correlations were observed. The statistically
significant correlations remained significant. For
example, the intraclass correlation for the general
satisfaction scale (holding the complexity, motor
skills, physical demands, and working conditions
variables constant) was .289, a minor change from the
.309 value observed earlier. Examination of the
correlations between the complexity, motor skills,
physical demands, and working conditions factors and the
satisfaction variables revealed no significant
relationships, which explains the very modest changes
observed in the intraclass correlations after
partialing. Discussion
The intraclass correlations of
.309 for general job satisfaction and .315 for intrinsic
satisfaction can be interpreted as broad heritabilities
or as the proportion of variance resulting from genetic
factors. The correlation computed for extrinsic
satisfaction was neither significantly different from
zero nor significantly different from the value obtained
for intrinsic satisfaction.
It is important to understand
that, although there is a significant genetic component
to intrinsic and general satisfaction, this component is
not overwhelming. Approximately 70% of the total
variance in this variable is explained by environmental
and other factors (e.g., error variance). The data are
supportive of the consistency model articulated by Staw
and Ross (1985). The hypothesis that the
heritability for intrinsic satisfaction would be
significantly higher than the heritability for extrinsic
satisfaction was not supported, but the data were in the
specified direction. A larger sample with greater power
might substantiate this prediction in the future.
It is interesting to speculate
as to why the one-item overall satisfaction measure
failed to demonstrate a significant heritability. One
possibility is that this item simply lacked sufficient
reliability. Another possible explanation is that the
variance of this item was somewhat constrained because
of a leniency effect (because it has the smallest
variance of any item on the inventory). A similar
question involves why there is a significant
heritability for certain items and not others. A
possible explanation is that these differences occurred
simply because of sampling error. That is, the various
intraclass correlations for the items differed only
because of chance fluctuations.
It is also appropriate to
comment on the tendencies of the MZ twins to hold
similar jobs, as indicated by their significant
intraclass correlations for complexity, motor skills,
and physical demands. These data are consistent with the
hypothesis that these twin pairs seek out environments
that are compatible with their particular genetic
makeups. This process might be mediated through
intellectual mechanisms. That is, relatively intelligent
individuals might seek out environments that are
relatively complex and challenging. This idea is
reinforced by the recent comment by McCormick
(1987) in which he suggests that “people tend to
gravitate into occupations that are compatible with
their own intellectual levels” (p. 45). Future research
needs to explore this and other mechanisms in
pinpointing the processes by which genetic elements
influence job choices.
What are the implications of
this research? First, it appears that the organization
may have somewhat less “control” over job satisfaction
than is commonly believed, particularly with respect to
intrinsic satisfaction. Although job enrichment efforts,
quality circles, and other environmental changes might
be made to enhance intrinsic job satisfaction, the data
suggest certain “boundaries” for each individual with
regard to job satisfaction. Individuals appear to bring
important predispositions to the job that may be more
difficult to modify than heretofore acknowledged. These
data, however, should not be construed to mean that job
enrichment and other interventions will have no effect;
they simply indicate that similar rank ordering on job
satisfaction variables of individuals placed in
different job environments are likely to obtain. Job
enrichment efforts may, however, have the intended
effect of raising mean levels of job satisfaction for
the individuals involved, even though rank ordering of
individuals is preserved.
A second implication of this
research is that prediction of future job satisfaction
or dissatisfaction with a different job may be possible
from knowledge of current satisfaction. This implication
is unique in two ways: First, we have not typically
considered job satisfaction as a criterion for
prediction using traditional selection methods (Pulakos
& Schmitt, 1983). Second, we have not
traditionally used job satisfaction as a predictor of
future satisfaction, although it would seem reasonable
to do so.
The ultimate goal of behavior
genetic research is to specify the mode of inheritance
and the mechanisms that lead to the expression of
specific phenotypes. It seems plausible that several
personality factors afford viable pathways, at least in
terms of explaining the mechanism for the heritability
of job satisfaction. For example, Tellegen
(1982) has developed a number of trait measures that
reflect “normal” personality factors. One factor,
labeled positive affect, reflects the capacity
that individuals have to experience job, enthusiasm, and
feelings of trust and gratification. Another factor,
negative affect, reflects a tendency to feel
fearful, worried, suspicious, and dissatisfied,
and to act in ways that perpetuate these feelings. Both
these factors have demonstrated high heritabilities on
the basis of twin studies (Tellegen
et al., 1988). It may be that the genetic pathway
operates via these, or similar, personality
dispositions. On the other hand, the relationships
between personality and job satisfaction may be limited
(cf. Furnham
& Zacherl, 1986), especially when the
individuals studied hold the same job or occupation.
There are several factors to
consider when evaluating these results. The modest
sample size and degree to which generalizations to the
nontwin population are allowable (on the basis of this
analysis) deserve comment. For example, the sample
studied includes a higher proportion of women than men
than is typically found in the general population. The
parents of monzygotic twins represent a random sample of
the population, but this may not be true for MZA twins.
Nevertheless, occupations included in this study
represent quite a diverse population of jobs, so that
some generalizability would appear to be warranted.
The research illustrates the
utility of using twins to study organizationally based
phenomena. Further research efforts might consider, for
example, studying twin pairs longitudinally across
different job environments to form additional estimates
of genetic and environmental contributions to such
variables as satisfaction and performance. We agree with
Staw
and Ross's (1985) contention that additional
research dealing with the influence of specific traits
(e.g., intelligence and personality) on job satisfaction
is needed. These efforts could aid us in disentangling
the various sources of variance that contribute to job
satisfaction and other organizationally based
phenomena. References
Arvey, R. D., & Gross, R. H.
(1977). Satisfaction levels and correlates of
satisfaction in the homemaker job. Journal of
Vocational Behavior, 10, 13–24.
Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (1984).
Twins reared apart and together: What they tell us about
human diversity. In S.Fox (Ed.), The chemical and
biological bases of individuality (pp. 147–184).
New York: Plenum.
Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (1987).
Diversity, development and determinism: A report on
identical twins reared apart. In M.Amelang (Ed.),
Bericht über den 35. Kongress der Deutschen
Gesellsschaft für Psychologie in Heidelberg 1986
[Proceedings of the 35th Congress of the German Society
for Psychology]. Göttingen, Federal Republic of Germany:
Hogrefe.
Bouchard, T. J., Jr., &
McGue, M. (1981). Familial studies of intelligence: A
review. Science, 212, 1055–1059.
Furnham, A., & Zacherl, M.
(1986). How people make their own environments: A theory
of genotype–environment effects. Child
Development, 54, 424–435.
Gerhart, B. (1987). How
important are dispositional factors as determinants of
job satisfaction? Implications for job design and other
personnel programs. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 72, 366–373.
Goldsmith, H. H. (1983). Genetic
influences on personality from infancy to adulthood.
Child Development, 54, 331–355.
Hay, D. A. (1985).
Essentials of behavior genetics. Melbourne,
Australia: Blackwell Scientific.
Locke, E. A. (1976). The nature
and causes of job satisfaction. In M. D.Durincetti
(Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational
psychology (pp. 1297–1349). Chicago: Rand
McNally.
Lykken, D. T. (1978). The
diagnosis of zygosity in twins. Behavior
Genetics, 8, 437–473.
Martin, N. G., Eaves, L. J.,
Heath, A. C., Jardine, R., Feingold, L. M., &
Eysenck, H. J. (1986). Transmission of social attitudes.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Science,
83, 4364–4368.
McCormick, E. J. (1987). Job
component validity: A reply to the letter from
metanalysis. The Industrial–Organizational
Psychologist, 25, 45–47.
McGue, M., & Bouchard, T.
J., Jr. (1984). Adjustment of twin data for the effects
of age and sex. Behavior Genetics, 14,
325–343.
McGue, M., & Bouchard, T.
J., Jr. (in press). Genetic and environmental
determinants of information processing and special
mental abilities: A twin analysis. In R. J.Sternberg
(Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human
intelligence.
Muchinsky, P. M. (1983).
Psychology applied to work: An introduction to
industrial and organizational psychology. Homewood,
IL: Dorsey Press.
Nichols, R. C. (1978). Twin
studies of ability, personality and interests.
Homo, 29, 158–173.
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C.,
& McClearn, G. E. (1980). Behavioral genetics: A
primer. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
Pulakos, E. D., & Schmitt,
N. (1983). A longitudinal study of a valence model
approach for the prediction of job satisfaction of new
employees. Journal of Applied Psychology,
68, 157–165.
Roos, P. A., & Treiman, D.
J. (1980). Worker functions and worker traits for the
1970 U.S. census classification. In A. R.Miller, D.
J.Treiman, P. S.Cain, & P. S.Roos (Eds.), Work,
jobs and occupations: A critical review of the
dictionary of occupational titles (Appendix F).
Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Rowe, D. C. (1987). Resolving
the person–situation debate: Invitation to an
interdisciplinary dialogue. American
Psychologist, 42, 218–227.
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K.
(1983). How people make their own environments: A theory
of genotype–environment effects. Child
Development, 54, 424–435.
Snedecor, G. W., & Cochran,
W. G. (1967). Statistical methods. Ames: Iowa
State University Press.
Staw, B. M., Bell, N. E., &
Clausen, J. A. (1986). The dispositional approach to job
attitudes: A lifetime longitudinal test. American
Science Quarterly, 31, 56–77.
Staw, B. M., & Ross, J.
(1985). Stability in the midst of change: A
dispositional approach to job attitudes. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 70, 469–480.
Steiger, J. H. (1980). Tests for
comparing elements of a correlation matrix.
Psychological Bulletin, 87,
245–251.
Teasdale, T. W., & Owen, D.
R. (1984). Heredity and familial environment in
intelligence and educational level—A sibling study.
Nature, 309, 620–622.
Tellegen, A. (1982). Brief
manual for The Differential Personality
Questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript, University
of Minnesota, Minneapolis.
Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T.,
Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Wilcox, K. J., Segal, N. L., &
Rich, S. (1988). Personality similarity in twins reared
apart and together. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 54, 1031–1039.
U.S. Department of Labor.
(1977). Dictionary of occupational titles (4th
ed.). Washington DC: Government Printing Office.
Weiss, D. J., Dawis, R. V.,
England, G. W., & Lofquist, L. H. (1967). Manual
for the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire..
Minnesota studies in vocational rehabilitation.
(XXII), Minneapolis: Industrial Relations Center,
University of Minnesota.
Wernimont, P. F. (1966).
Intrinsic and extrinsic factors in job satisfaction.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 50,
41–50.
Willerman, L. (1979). The
psychology of individual and group differences. San
Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
Submitted: June 18,
1987 Revised: March 28, 1988 Accepted:
March 10, 1988
Copyright 1989 American
Psychological Association This publication is
protected by US and international copyright laws
and its content may not be copied without the
copyright holders express written permission
except for the print or download capabilities of
the retrieval software used for access. This
content is intended solely for the use of the
individual
user.
Source: Journal
of Applied Psychology. Vol.74 (2) pp.
187-192. Accession
Number: apl-74-2-187 Digital
Object
Identifier: 10.1037/0021-9010.74.2.187 | |