By: Timothy A. Judge
Department of Management, University of
Florida Joyce E. Bono
Department of Psychology, University of
Minnesota Amir Erez
Department of Management, University of
Florida Edwin A. Locke
Department of Management, University of Maryland
(College Park)
Correspondence concerning this
article should be addressed to: Timothy A. Judge,
Department of Management, University of Florida, 211D
Box 117165, Gainesville, FL 32611 Electronic Mail may be
sent to: tjudge@ufl.edu.
Recently, considerable research
attention has centered on a broad personality trait
termed core self-evaluations. Introduced by Judge,
Locke, and Durham (1997), the core self-evaluations
concept represents the fundamental assessments that
people make about their worthiness, competence, and
capabilities; such evaluations vary from positive to
negative self-appraisals. In their original development
of the concept, Judge et al. identified three
fundamental, broad, and self-evaluative traits
(self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy, and
neuroticism) that indicated core self-evaluations. They
suggested that locus of control might qualify as a core
trait; therefore, most subsequent research includes all
four core traits. In the past 6 years, there have been
16 investigations of core self-evaluations. Although the
core self-evaluations concept has been related to
several criteria—including motivation (Erez
& Judge, 2001), job performance (Judge
& Bono, 2001), stress (Best,
2003), and leadership (Eisenberg,
2000)—the most commonly investigated criterion is
job satisfaction. The studies that have investigated the
relationship between core self-evaluations to job
satisfaction have shown that there is a relationship
between the two concepts (Judge
& Bono, 2001), shedding light on the processes
by which individuals with a positive self-regard are
more satisfied with their jobs (Judge,
Bono, & Locke, 2000; Judge,
Locke, Durham, & Kluger, 1998). However, these
studies have focused on only one specific mediating
factor—intrinsic job characteristics—and have not tested
a theoretical framework that might further explain
psychologically how and why those with positive core
self-evaluations are more satisfied with their jobs. As
Judge,
Bono, Erez, Locke, and Thoresen (2002) commented,
“Other theoretical mechanisms will need to be studied to
more fully understand the nature of the relationship
between core self-evaluations and job satisfaction” (p.
70).
One psychological mechanism that
may link core self-evaluations to job satisfaction is
the way in which people choose goals. A growing body of
research suggests that people who choose goals that are
concordant with their ideals, interests, and values are
happier than those who pursue goals for other (e.g.,
extrinsic or defensive) reasons (see Sheldon
& Elliot, 1998). In addition, several authors
(Elliot
& Sheldon, 1998; Elliot,
Sheldon, & Church, 1997) found that people who
perceive themselves positively (i.e., high self-esteem,
low neuroticism) tend to pursue self-concordant goals to
a greater extent than people with a negative self-view.
On the basis of this research, Judge
and Larsen (2001) have suggested that positive
individuals may also be more likely to evoke and pursue
approach work goals (i.e., goals that entail
moving toward a positive outcome or state; Elliot
et al., 1997) or accomplishment through the
attainment of aspirations (Shah
& Higgins, 2001). In contrast, they argued that
negative individuals should be more likely to pursue
avoidance or prevention goals (goals that entail moving
away from a negative outcome or state or averting a
negative result; Elliot
et al., 1997; Shah
& Higgins, 2001). Thus, one mechanism that may
link core self-evaluations (positive self-regard) and
job satisfaction is the motivation underlying goal
pursuit such that approach goals are likely to lead to
satisfaction, and avoidance goals are more likely to
lead to dissatisfaction (Roberson,
1990).
In summary, the arguments
advanced by goal researchers (i.e., Elliot
& Sheldon, 1998; Elliot
et al., 1997) are that people with a positive
self-regard tend to pursue self-concordant goals and
that these self-concordant goals make them happy. In
parallel fashion, industrial and organizational (I/O)
psychologists argue that people with positive
self-regard tend to be more satisfied with their work
and life (Judge
& Bono, 2001; Judge
et al., 1998, 2000).
However, these two lines of research have not yet been
combined in a meaningful manner. Thus, the overall
purpose of this research was to test a model that brings
together these two theoretical models—the core
self-evaluations model and the self-concordance
model (Sheldon
& Elliot, 1998), a model explaining the approach
and avoidance processes underlying goal pursuit—in
further understanding the dispositional source of job
satisfaction. In the next section of the article, we
describe the theoretical concepts that are at focus
here: core self-evaluations and self-concordance. We
then introduce a hypothesized model and provide
theoretical support for linkages in the model. Core
Self-Evaluations, Self-Concordance, and Hypothesized
Models
Core Self-Evaluations Model
According to Judge
et al. (1997), the core self-evaluations concept is
a higher order trait representing the fundamental
evaluations that people make about themselves and their
worthiness, competence, and capability. In the core
self-evaluations theory, the core concept is indicated
by four traits: self-esteem, locus of control,
neuroticism, and generalized self-efficacy. Self-esteem
can be defined as the overall value that one places on
oneself as a person (Harter,
1990). Generalized self-efficacy is an appraisal of
how well one can handle life's challenges (Locke,
McClear, & Knight, 1996). Neuroticism is the
tendency to have a negativistic outlook and to focus on
negative aspects of the self (Watson,
2000). Finally, locus of control is concerned with
beliefs about the causes of events in one's life—locus
is internal when individuals see outcomes as being
contingent on their own behavior (Rotter,
1966).
In considering the relationships
among these traits, it is worth noting that self-esteem,
locus of control, and neuroticism (also known as
emotional stability or emotional adjustment) are the
most widely studied personality concepts in
psychology—cumulatively, the traits have been the
subject of more than 50,000 studies (Judge
& Bono, 2001). Despite the prominence of these
traits, and some rather obvious connections among them,
few investigations have included more than a single core
trait. Recently, in the most thorough analysis of the
traits to date, Judge,
Erez, Bono, and Thoresen (2002) found that an
overall core self-evaluations factor could be extracted
from the correlations among the four traits, and that
this common factor was an important positive predictor
of life satisfaction and a negative predictor of stress,
strain, and depression. Moreover, Judge, Erez, et al.
found that the individual core traits were highly
related, displayed quite similar patterns of
correlations with other variables, and failed to add
incremental validity beyond the common core factor. That
the individual core traits fail to add incremental
validity beyond the common core factor provides support
for the validity of the core self-evaluations concept,
suggesting that the variance attributable to each
individual core trait is less important than the
variance these traits share in common (which represents
core self-evaluations).
In trying to explain the
relationship between core self-evaluations and job
satisfaction noted earlier, Judge
et al. (1998) found that the link was mediated by
perceptions of intrinsic job characteristics. For
example, individuals with a positive self-regard were
more likely to perceive their jobs as interesting,
significant, and autonomous than individuals with
negative self-regard. Expanding on this explanation, Judge
et al. (2000) demonstrated that individuals with
positive self-regard, measured in early childhood, not
only perceived their job as more intrinsically
satisfying, but also were more likely to hold more
complex jobs. In turn, choosing more complex jobs was
associated with increased levels of job satisfaction.
Thus, this study provided both perceptual and behavioral
explanations for the link between core self-evaluations
and job satisfaction. However, it did not account for,
nor has other research accounted for, the psychological
or cognitive mechanisms that underlie these
relationships. Similarly, although Judge
et al. (2002) and Judge,
Erez, Bono, and Thoresen (2003) found strong
relationships between core self-evaluations and life
satisfaction, no theoretical explanations were given to
account for these relationships. In this study, we
attempt to illuminate these psychological mechanisms by
using the self-concordance model.
Self-Concordance Model
Goals have played an important
role in psychology. In I/O psychology, the performance
implications of goal setting are well documented (Locke
& Latham, 2002). In the subjective well-being
literature, goals have been thought of as personal
strivings (Emmons,
1992). Recent research on how goals contribute to
well-being has been conducted by Sheldon
and Elliot (1998, 1999)
under the auspices of the self-concordance theory.
Self-concordance theory, derived from Deci
and Ryan's (1985) self-determination theory,
predicts that individuals are happiest when stated goals
match enduring interests and values. The authors argue
that self-concordance leads to well-being because (a) it
enables individuals to put effort into goals, thus
increasing the probability of goal attainment, and (b)
people are more likely to have attained the goals that
will make them happy (because the identified and
intrinsic goals better fulfill an individual's enduring
needs, interests, and values).
The self-concordance model
argues that individuals may pursue a goal for one or
more of four types of reasons (Sheldon
& Elliot, 1998):
-
External: pursuing a goal that is due to others'
wishes or to attain rewards that indirectly satisfy
needs or interests (e.g., performing a task to earn
money);
-
Introjected: pursuing a goal to avoid feelings of
shame, guilt, or anxiety (e.g., organizing one's files
out of a sense of guilt or obligation);
-
Identified: pursuing a goal out of a belief that
it is an intrinsically important goal to have (e.g.,
helping a coworker with a computer problem out of a
belief that it is important to help other employees);
and
-
Intrinsic: pursuing a goal because of the fun and
enjoyment it provides (e.g., setting aside time to
chat with a coworker because one finds the
conversation personally engaging).
There are several points worth
noting with respect to the self-concordance model.
First, these reasons are not argued to be mutually
exclusive; individuals may pursue a goal for several
reasons. Second, Sheldon
and Elliot (1998) argued that goals pursued for
identified or intrinsic reasons represent autonomous
motives because they emanate from self-choices that
reflect deeply held personal values, whereas goals
pursued for extrinsic or introjected reasons represent
controlled motives because they emanate from forces
outside the self to which the person does not give full
assent (Elliot
et al., 1997, have equated autonomous motives with
approach goals and controlled motives with
avoidance goals). Third, in self-concordance
research, goals are not objectively classifiable.
Rather, two individuals may pursue the same goal for
different reasons. Thus, in order to measure
self-concordance, one must ask people about their
reasons for pursuing various goals rather than assuming
certain goals per se are self-concordant or not
self-concordant. Of course, it is true that some goals
are inherently more likely to be self-concordant for
most individuals than are others (e.g., reading an
important book vs. serving on a dull, unimportant
committee or task force).
Research on the self-concordance
model has been supportive. Sheldon
and Elliot (1999) found that self-concordant motives
are more likely to lead to well-being. According to
these authors, when a person strives because of strong
interest or because of self-identified personal
convictions, the goals are held to be well integrated
with the self. Self-concordant goals are likely to
receive sustained effort over time, be more attainable,
and as such are more satisfying. In contrast, goals
pursued only because of external pressure or because of
feelings of guilt and anxiety are assumed to come from
nonintegrated areas of the self. Because they are less
integrated and representative of stable interests, the
motivational strength behind non-self-concordant goals
is likely to fade, thus making them less attainable and
less satisfying. Indeed, in three separate studies,
Sheldon and Elliot found that self-concordant goals were
more sustainable and more attainable, which led to
increased levels of subjective well-being. Moreover,
there is also evidence that self-concordance leads to an
upward spiral of well-being, which in turn leads to
stronger effects over time (Sheldon
& Houser-Marko, 2001).
Although prior research has
linked goal self-concordance to happiness (e.g., Sheldon
& Houser-Marko, 2001) and a broad subjective
well-being composite that includes both affect and life
satisfaction (e.g., Sheldon
& Elliot, 1999), no research has specifically
tested the self-concordance model with respect to life
satisfaction per se. More important, no research has
tested self-concordance as a mediator of the
relationship between core self-evaluations and life
satisfaction. Similarly, given the broad support for the
self-concordance model, it is somewhat surprising that
little published research has studied the model in a
work context in general or linked it to job satisfaction
in particular. If self-concordant goals do indeed lead
to an increased level of happiness and satisfaction with
the job and life, it seems important to identify the
antecedents to the adoption of the different types of
goals. However, with only few exceptions (Sheldon
& Elliot, 1999), very little work has been
conducted to identify these antecedents. We argue in the
next section of the article that the common core of the
four dispositional traits of self-esteem, neuroticism,
generalized self-efficacy, and locus of control should
be considered as one of the major antecedents for
choosing self-concordant goals.
Hypothesized Model
Figure
1 displays the hypothesized model linking core
self-evaluations to self-concordant goals, goal
attainment, and job satisfaction. The model posits,
consistent with prior research (Judge
et al., 1998, 2000),
that the core self-evaluations concept is indicated by
self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of
control, and neuroticism. As shown in Figure
1, the loadings of the first three traits onto the
latent factor should be positive, whereas the loading of
neuroticism should be negative.

Figure
1. Hypothesized model linking core self-evaluations,
goal self-concordance, goal attainment, and job/life
satisfaction. Dashed lines indicate mediating role of
goal attainment tested in separate model
The structural portion of the
model includes a link from core self-evaluations to
self-concordant goals. On the basis of prior
self-concordance research (e.g., Sheldon
& Elliot, 1998, 1999),
we define goals as self-concordant based on the degree
to which they are pursued for autonomous (intrinsic and
identified) reasons and the absence of their pursuit for
controlled (extrinsic and introjected) reasons.
Individuals with positive self-regard think of
themselves as worthy, capable, and competent and
therefore should be less influenced by external or
introjected pressures. In effect, individuals with high
self-esteem, the central component of core
self-evaluations, have been shown to be less “plastic”
in their behavioral responses to social influence and
feedback (Brockner,
1988). As such, individuals with positive core
self-evaluations should be more likely to choose
self-concordant goals. In contrast, individuals with
negative self-regard are likely to be more influenced by
anxiety and guilt and should therefore be more likely to
choose controlled (introjected and external) goals (Elliot
et al., 1997). Thus, individuals with a positive
self-concept are more likely to evoke and pursue
approach (identified and intrinsic) work goals, whereas
negative individuals are more likely to evoke and pursue
avoidance (extrinsic and introjected) goals, thereby
leading individuals with positive core self-evaluations
to greater goal self-concordance.
Indeed, some empirical research
has linked several of the individual core traits to
aspects of self-concordance. Elliot
and Sheldon (1997) found that avoidance goals were
negatively related to self-esteem, and other research
has shown that neuroticism is positively related to such
goals (Elliot
& Sheldon, 1998; Elliot
et al., 1997). Furthermore, neuroticism has been
shown to be negatively correlated with perceived
autonomy and positively correlated with “controlledness”
(Elliot
& Sheldon, 1998). Thus, the combined conceptual
and empirical evidence suggests that core
self-evaluations will be positively related to goal
self-concordance.
Hypothesis 1 (H-1): Core self-evaluations
will be positively related to goal
self-concordance.
No previous research has been
published linking the self-concordance model to job or
life satisfaction. However, there is ample evidence that
self-concordance is positively associated with
subjective well-being (Elliot
& Sheldon, 1997, 1998;
Elliot
et al., 1997; Sheldon
& Elliot, 1999). Given this body of evidence,
and the strong relationship between job satisfaction,
life satisfaction, and happiness (Judge
& Hulin, 1993; Tait,
Padgett, & Baldwin, 1989), it seems likely that
self-concordant goals will be positively related to job
and life satisfaction. It is possible, however, to
explicate the relationship between goal self-concordance
and job and life satisfaction even further.
Specifically, goal attainment should mediate, at least
in part, the relationship between goal self-concordance
and job and life satisfaction.
The first link in this
mediational relationship requires that self-concordance
leads to goal attainment. In the self-concordance model,
self-concordant goals are more likely to be attained
because individuals put forth more effort toward goals
that are consistent with their enduring needs and values
(Sheldon
& Elliot, 1998). Empirical evidence supports the
link between self-concordance and goal attainment (Sheldon
& Elliot, 1998; Sheldon
& Houser-Marko, 2001). A recent review of the
available studies revealed that all studies of the
relationship between goal self-concordance and goal
progress are positive, with ¯d =.37 (Koestner,
Lekes, Powers, & Chicoine, 2002). If a similar
situation operates with respect to work goals, then work
goal self-concordance should be positively related to
work goal attainment.
The second part of the
mediational relationship requires that goal attainment
be related to job and life satisfaction. Indeed,
evidence indicates that goal attainment is positively
related to satisfaction (Elliot
et al., 1997; Emmons
& Diener, 1986). As Sheldon
and Elliot (1999) noted, “There are natural
satisfactions to be found in the process of exercising
one's competencies to move toward desired outcomes” (p.
484). Similarly, in the goal setting literature, “Goal
success is viewed as leading to self-satisfaction” (Locke,
1991, p. 294). Maier
and Brunstein (2001) found that progress toward goal
accomplishment was positively related to job
satisfaction. Koestner
et al.'s (2002) review demonstrated that all studies
on the relationship between goal progress to changes in
well-being were also positive, with ¯d =.61.
Thus, attainment of work and life goals should result in
individuals being more satisfied with their jobs and
lives (where the goals are attained). Although various
hypothesized links are embedded in Figure
1 (e.g., a link from self-concordance to goal
attainment), the key hypothesized links are as follows:
Hypothesis 2 (H-2): Goal self-concordance
will partly mediate the relationship between core
self-evaluations and job/life
satisfaction.
Hypothesis 3 (H-3): Goal attainment will
partly mediate the relationship between
self-concordance and job/life
satisfaction.
Finally, included in Figure
1 are direct links from core self-evaluations and
self-concordance to job/life satisfaction. Though we
believe that self-concordance will mediate the
relationship between core self-evaluations and job
satisfaction, and that goal attainment will mediate the
relationship between self-concordance and job/life
satisfaction, we do not believe that the mediation
effect will be total. Intrinsic job characteristics have
been shown to mediate the relationship between core
self-evaluations and job satisfaction (Judge
et al., 1998, 2000).
Furthermore, there are other factors that may mediate
the relationship between core self-evaluations and job
satisfaction, such as actions people take on the job and
the decisions people make (Judge
et al., 1997), that have not been tested in previous
research and are not the focus here. Similarly, in
self-concordance research, self-concordance is thought
to lead to satisfaction for reasons other than goal
attainment (the intrinsically satisfying nature of goal
pursuit; Sheldon
& Elliot, 1999). For these reasons, we expect
partial but not complete mediation. Overview
of Studies
Because no previous research has
examined the mediating role of self-concordance in the
relationship between core self-evaluations and job/life
satisfaction, we conducted two studies. First, we
examined the mediating role of self-concordance and goal
attainment with respect to the personal goals of a
prototypic sample in self-concordance research:
undergraduate college students. However, to extend these
results even further, we study self-concordance in the
work situation. Specifically, in Study 2, we test a
model parallel to that in Study 1, but focusing on work
goals and job satisfaction (as opposed to personal goals
and life satisfaction). Taken together, these results
should determine whether self-concordance (and goal
attainment) mediates the relationship between core
self-evaluations and job/life satisfaction and whether
it is productive to apply the self-concordance framework
to the work setting. Study
1
Method
Participants and
Procedures
Participants were 240
undergraduate management students (54% men; average age
20.4 years) at a southeastern U.S. university who
participated in the study for extra credit. Participants
completed two surveys, 2 months apart. Personality and
self-concordance were measured at Time 1, and goal
attainment and life satisfaction were measured at Time
2. One hundred and eighty-three of the original 240
participants (76%) responded to the Time 2 survey. No
significant differences between respondents and
nonrespondents were found for age, sex, or any of the
study variables.
Measures
Core
self-evaluations
Consistent with previous core
self-evaluations research (e.g., Judge
et al., 1998), the core self-evaluations concept was
measured with four scales. Self-esteem was assessed with
the 10-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg,
1965). Items include “On the whole, I am satisfied
with myself” and “I wish I could have more respect for
myself” (reverse scored). Generalized self-efficacy was
assessed with 7 items developed by Judge et al. Sample
items include “I avoid trying to learn new things when
they look too difficult for me” and “New jobs are
usually well within the scope of my abilities.” Locus of
control was assessed with 6 items from the Internality
scale of Levenson's
(1981) Internality, Powerful Others, and Chance
(IPC) Scale. Sample items include “When I get what I
want, it is usually because I worked hard for it” and
“My life is determined by my own actions.” Finally,
neuroticism was measured using the 12-item Eysenck
Personality Inventory (Eysenck
& Eysenck, 1968) Neuroticism scale. Items in
this scale include “I am a nervous person” and “I am a
worrier.” Responses for all four personality scales were
anchored on a 5-point scale with responses ranging from
1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly
agree). The items within each scale were averaged
to form a single score for self-esteem, generalized
self-efficacy, locus of control, and neuroticism.
Consistent with prior practices (Judge
et al., 1998), the four scales were then treated as
indicators of a higher order core self-evaluations
concept.
Self-concordance
We used Sheldon and colleagues'
goal-based measure of self-concordance (Sheldon
& Elliot, 1998; Sheldon
& Kasser, 1998). Participants were asked to
record six of their short-term goals. Consistent with
Sheldon and Elliot, we asked participants to reflect on
their current life goals—goals that could reasonably be
attained in the next 60 days. (We chose 60 days because,
consistent with other self-concordance research that has
utilized a similar time period [e.g., Elliot
& Sheldon, 1997], it was a reasonable time
period over which individuals could set and attain
work-related goals.) After identifying their goals,
participants were asked to report their reasons for goal
pursuit, for each goal separately. Each of the six goals
was followed by questions representing the four types of
motivation that comprise the self-concordance construct
(external, introjected, identified, and intrinsic). To
measure the motives underlying goal pursuit, we used
items developed by Sheldon and Elliot. Participants
responded to each of the items for each of their six
goals. Consistent with Sheldon and Elliot, responses
were on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all for this
reason) to 9 (completely for this reason).
Responses were averaged across the six goals to form a
single score for external, introjected, identified, and
intrinsic motivation. We followed steps used by Sheldon
and Elliot to form a self-concordance composite, adding
together the Intrinsic and Identified Scales and
subtracting the External and Introjected Scales. We
calculated the reliability of the composite using
procedures recommended by Hunter
and Schmidt (1990).
Goal attainment
We used two items from prior
self-concordance research (Sheldon
& Elliot, 1999) as a self-report of goal
attainment. The items are “I have made considerable
progress toward attaining this goal” and “I accomplished
what I set out to do with this goal.” Participants
responded to each of these items for each of their six
goals, after 2 months. The same 5-point scale used for
core self-evaluations was used for goal attainment.
Responses were averaged across items and goals to form a
goal attainment score.
Life satisfaction
Life satisfaction was measured
with the five-item Satisfaction With Life Scale (Diener,
Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985). The five items
are “In most ways my life is close to ideal,” “The
conditions of my life are excellent,” “I am satisfied
with my life,” “So far I have gotten the important
things I want in life,” and “If I could live my life
over, I would change almost nothing.” Participants used
the same 5-point scale used for job satisfaction. Items
were averaged to form a single life satisfaction
score.
Analyses and Results
Correlations among the variables
are presented in Table
1. To test the hypotheses, we used structural
equation modeling (LISREL 8.3; Jöreskog
& Sörbom, 1993) to estimate a series of models.
Figure
2 presents the results of a model testing H-1 and
H-2. The fit statistics of the hypothesized
model—χ2(12, N = 183) = 28.17,
p <.01; root mean square error of
approximation (RMSEA) =.08; root mean square residual
(RMSR) =.09; Comparative Fit Index (CFI) =.97; IFI
(Incremental Fit Index) =.97—show an adequate fit to the
data. In this model, the link between core
self-evaluations and self-concordant goal pursuit was
significant (
=.24, p <.01), supporting H-1. In addition,
the link between self-concordance and life satisfaction
was also significant (
=.26, p <.01). Examination of the direct and
indirect effects of core self-evaluations on life
satisfaction revealed a total effect of.54 (p
<.01), with an indirect effect (through
self-concordant goal pursuit) of.06 (p
<.05), supporting H-2. However, because the indirect
link was small, we tested an alternative model in which
self-concordant goals do not mediate the core
self-evaluation-life satisfaction link. That is, in this
alternative model we dropped a link from self-concordant
goal pursuit to life satisfaction. The overall fit
statistics for this alternative were lower than those of
the hypothesized model: χ2(13, N =
183) = 39.25, p <.01; RMSEA =.11; RMSR =.10;
CFI =.94; IFI =.95. Chi-square differences between the
hypothesized and the alternative models,
Δχ2(1, N = 183)= 11.08, p
<.01, also showed a significantly poorer fit of the
alternative model to the data. Thus, we conclude that
some, though not all, of the association between core
self-evaluations and life satisfaction were mediated by
self-concordance, supporting H-2.

Means,
Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations Among Study 1
Variables

Figure
2. Study 1: Relationships among core self-evaluations,
goal self-concordance, and life satisfaction. n = 183.
**p <.01
To test H-3, we added goal
attainment to our model. Overall fit statistics of the
hypothesized model were good, Δχ2(10,
N = 180) = 14.69, p <.10; RMSEA
=.05; RMSR =.04; CFI =.99; IFI =.99. Results presented
in Figure
3 reveal a significant link between goal attainment
and life satisfaction (
=.20, p <.05). However, the link between
self-concordant goals and goal attainment was only
marginally significant:
=.18, p <.10, with t(1) = 1.95. The
indirect link from self-concordant goals to life
satisfaction through goal attainment was not significant
(.05), thus, H-3 was not supported. Although not
officially hypothesized, we also tested the link from
core self-evaluation to goal attainment through goal
self-concordance. This indirect effect (.06, p
<.05) revealed that self-concordant goals fully
mediated the relationship between core self-evaluations
and goal attainment.

Figure
3. Study 1: Model testing the mediating role of goal
attainment in relationship between goal self-concordance
and life satisfaction. n = 183. †p <.10. *p <.05.
**p <.01
Brief Discussion
Results from Study 1 revealed
that self-concordance partly mediated the relationship
between core self-evaluations and life satisfaction.
Though previous research has linked self-concordance to
subjective well-being (e.g., Sheldon
& Elliot, 1999), no previous research has
investigated whether it mediates the relationship
between core self-evaluations and life satisfaction.
This mediational relationship is significant because
life satisfaction was measured several months after core
self-evaluations and self-concordance were measured.
Additionally, it appears that core self-evaluation also
affected goal attainment through its effect on goal
self-concordance. Thus, it seems that those who have
positive self-evaluations tend to choose self-concordant
goals, and this choice, in turn, results in goal
attainment and life satisfaction. However, goal
attainment did not explain the relationship between goal
self-concordance and life satisfaction. Thus, we can
conclude that part of the reason that individuals with a
positive self-concept are happier is because they are
more likely to set self-concordant goals. We can also
conclude that they are more likely to attain their
personal goals. However, we cannot conclude that the
reason that people with positive self-concept are more
satisfied with their lives is because they attain those
goals.
What this study does not reveal,
of course, is whether a similar mediational relationship
will be found with respect to job satisfaction.
Moreover, as with most previous self-concordance
research, individuals in our sample are young adults in
college. Though evidence indicates that age is weakly
related to life satisfaction, individuals do adjust
their goals as they age (Diener,
Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). Moreover, it is
possible that goals and therefore the self-concordance
process is different for working adults. Because
previous self-concordance research has focused on
personal goals (Koestner
et al., 2002; Sheldon
& Elliot, 1998, 1999;
Sheldon
& Houser-Marko, 2001), it is important to
investigate self-concordance and goal attainment in a
work context. This is the focus of Study 2. Study
2
Method
Pilot Study
Because the self-concordance
measure used in past research has not been studied in a
work context, before testing the hypothesized model
involving work self-concordance, we sought to
investigate the validity of the measure. Specifically,
we administered the measure to 156 individuals who were
members of a national management association. Because
previous self-concordance research by Sheldon, Elliot,
and colleagues has been based on college students in
reference to life goals (e.g., Sheldon
& Elliot, 1998), it was necessary to make some
modifications to their measures. Specifically,
participants were asked about work goals in the next 60
days. Other aspects of the measure were the same as
Study 1 (see also description in Study 2). The results
revealed that the self-concordance facets were
relatively reliable (intrinsic, α =.80; identified, α
=.70; introjected, α =.83; external, α =.82). Moreover,
the overall self-concordance composite was also reliable
(α =.83). These results suggest that it is possible to
measure work goal self-concordance well.
Participants and
Procedures
Participants were employees of a
large defense contractor in the southwestern region of
the United States; a manufacturing plant, also in the
Southwest; and a small financial services organization
in the Midwest whose supervisors participated in a study
of leadership effectiveness (Bono
& Judge, 2003). Participants completed two
surveys on the Internet. The first survey included the
personality and self-concordance items. An e-mail with
an Internet link was sent to remind participants to
complete a follow-up survey after 60 days. The Time 2
survey contained goal attainment and job satisfaction
items. Surveys were received from 77% (260) of employees
at Time 1. Of those who responded at Time 1, 72% also
completed a survey at Time 2, resulting in a Time 1-Time
2 matched data set of 251 employees. These employees had
been working for their current supervisor for 2 years,
on average. Data from the two organizations (n
= 65 for the financial services company and n =
186 for the manufacturing company) were combined into a
single sample because data were collected using the same
procedures, mean levels of key variables (core
self-evaluations, goal achievement, self-concordance,
and job satisfaction) were not significantly different
between the two organizations, and the pattern of
associations between key variables was consistent across
organizations. Combining the two samples also increases
our confidence that results from this study will
generalize to diverse organizations.
Measures
Core
self-evaluations
Self-esteem, generalized
self-efficacy, and locus of control were measured with
the same scales as in Study 1 (the Rosenberg,
1965, Judge
et al., 1998, and modified Levenson,
1981, scales, respectively). Neuroticism was
measured using the 12-item NEO-FFI (Costa
& McCrae, 1992). Sample items include “I rarely
feel fearful or anxious” (reverse scored) and “I often
feel helpless and want someone else to solve my
problems.” As with Study 1, responses were evaluated on
a 1–5 response scale, items within each scale were
averaged, and then the four scales were treated as
indicators of the latent core self-evaluations
concept.
Self-concordance
On the basis of the results of
the pilot study, individuals were asked to identify the
work goals that they would pursue over the next 60 days.
After identifying their work goals, participants
reported their reasons for each goal. Each goal was
followed by questions representing the four types of
motivation that comprise the self-concordance construct
(external, introjected, identified, and intrinsic). To
measure the motives underlying goal pursuit, we adapted
items developed by Sheldon
and Elliot (1998). The items are “I pursue this goal
because I really believe it is an important goal to
have” (identified); “I pursue this goal because of the
fun and enjoyment it provides” (intrinsic); “I pursue
this goal because I would feel guilty, anxious, or
ashamed if I did not” (introjected); and “I pursue this
goal because somebody else wants me to or because the
situation demands it” (external). Participants responded
to each statement for each of their six goals. As in
Study 1, responses were anchored on a 1–9 scale with
responses ranging from 1 (not at all for this
reason) to 9 (completely for this reason)
and were then averaged across the six goals to form a
single score for external, introjected, identified, and
intrinsic motivation. We then followed Sheldon
and Elliot (1998) in forming the self-concordance
composite.
Goal attainment
We used two items adapted from
prior self-concordance research (Sheldon
& Elliot, 1999) as a self-report of goal
attainment. The items are “I have made considerable
progress toward attaining this goal” and “I accomplished
what I set out to do with this goal.” Participants
responded to each of these items for each of their six
goals, after 2 months. The same 5-point scale used for
core self-evaluations was used for goal attainment.
Responses were averaged across items and goals to form a
goal attainment score.
Job satisfaction
Job satisfaction was measured
with the short form of the Brayfield
and Rothe (1951) Job Satisfaction Scale. Items are
“Most days I am enthusiastic about my work,” “I feel
fairly satisfied with my present job,” “Each day at work
seems like it will never end” (reverse scored), “I find
real enjoyment in my work,” and “I consider my job
rather unpleasant” (reverse scored). Responses were
evaluated on the same 5-point scale used for personality
and goal attainment. The five items were averaged to
form a job satisfaction score.
Analyses and Results
Because data in this study were
collected from employees of two organizations, we
compared mean levels of variables between organizations.
There were no significant mean level differences between
the two companies in employee personality, goal
self-concordance, or goal attainment. The mean level of
job satisfaction in the two companies was significantly
different. However, the differences were small (3.81 and
4.00 for the manufacturing and the financial services
organizations, respectively).
Zero-order correlations among
the variables are presented in Table
2. Our first model, testing H-1 and H-2, is
presented in Figure
4. Fit statistics for this model were acceptable:
χ2(12, N = 251)= 31.20, p
<.01; RMSEA =.07; RMSR =.07; CFI =.97; IFI =.97. The
core self-evaluations latent factor was significantly
related to self-concordance (
=.29, p <.01), supporting H-1. We also found
an indirect link between core self-evaluations and job
satisfaction mediated by self-concordance (.06,
p <.05). An alternative no mediation model
(which dropped a link from self-concordant goal pursuit
to job satisfaction) provided poorer fit statistics for
the data overall: χ2(13, N = 251) =
37.72, p <.01; RMSEA =.09; RMSR =.07; CFI
=.96; IFI =.96. Chi-square differences between the
hypothesized and the alternative model,
Δχ2(1, N = 251) = 6.52 (p
<.05) also revealed a significant drop. Thus, we also
found that self-concordance mediated the core
self-evaluations-satisfaction link, supporting H-2.

Means,
Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations Among Study 2
Variables

Figure
4. Study 2: Relationships among core self-evaluations,
goal self-concordance, and job satisfaction. n = 251. *p
<.05. **p <.01
To test H-3, we added goal
attainment to the model (see Figure
5). The overall fit statistics were good,
χ2(10, N = 251)= 21.82, p
<.01; RMSEA =.07; RMSR =.04; CFI =.98; IFI =.98.
There was a significant link between self-concordant
goals and goal attainment (
=.22, p <.05). However, in this study, the
link between goal attainment and job satisfaction was
not significant (
=.10, p >.05). The indirect link between
self-concordant goals and job satisfaction through goal
attainment was not significant (.02). Therefore, as in
Study 1, H-3 was not supported. However, examination of
the other indirect effects revealed that self-concordant
goals did fully mediate the link between core
self-evaluations and goal attainment (.06, p
<.05).

Figure
5. Study 2: Model testing the mediating role of goal
attainment in relationship between goal self-concordance
and job satisfaction. n = 251. *p <.05. **p
<.01
Brief Discussion
Results from Study 2 provided
some support for the hypothesized model. Specifically,
the core self-evaluations latent factor was related to
goal self-concordance, and goal self-concordance was
related to job satisfaction and goal attainment.
However, goal attainment was not related to job
satisfaction. These findings also show that
self-concordance can be measured with respect to work
goals and that self-concordant work goals are linked to
core self-evaluations and goal attainment. Moreover,
work goal self-concordance mediates part of the
relationship between core self-evaluations and job
satisfaction. These results add to Study 1 by showing
that work goal self-concordance mediates the link
between core self-evaluations and job satisfaction
(Study 2) in much the same way as personal goal
self-concordance mediates the relationship between core
self-evaluations and life satisfaction (Study 1).
In summary, results of these
analyses provided support for the hypothesized model
with respect to the role of self-concordant goals in
predicting life satisfaction (Study 1) and job
satisfaction (Study 2). Notably, though, the direct
links were stronger than the indirect links. Judging
from the results in the correlation matrix in this study
as well as in Study 1 (see Table
1), it appears that autonomous (intrinsic and
identified) motives underlying goal pursuit are the
motives most correlated with job satisfaction.
Accordingly, we estimated models using autonomous
(intrinsic plus identified) goal pursuit in place of the
overall self-concordance composite variable. Although
the results with autonomous goal pursuit were slightly
stronger in magnitude, the results were quite similar,
and in no case did the significance of a variable
change. Accordingly, consistent with the large body of
research on self-concordance in the personality
literature (Elliot
& Sheldon, 1998; Elliot
et al., 1997; Koestner
et al., 2002; Sheldon
& Elliot, 1998, 1999;
Sheldon
& Houser-Marko, 2001; Sheldon
& Kasser, 1998), we retained the overall
self-concordance composite. General
Discussion
In the past decade, two
significant trends have emerged in personality
psychology. First, although vendors of folk wisdom have
inscribed for decades the virtues of positive thinking
(see Peale,
1952), only recently have researchers started to
methodologically investigate the consequences of
positivity (see Seligman
& Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Second, as noted by
Locke
(1997), researchers have begun to investigate the
importance of personal goals as the central organizers
of affect (Sheldon
& Elliot, 1999), which complements research on
the positive consequence of goals. Despite the
relatively short time that these two research trends
have existed, they have produced some impressive results
(see Diener,
2000, for review). The hypothesized model,
investigated in this study, combines these two promising
approaches to human motivation. According to this model,
people with positive self-regard are more likely to have
self-concordant goals. In turn, those with more
self-concordant goals—goals that reflect feelings of
intrinsic interest and identity congruence rather than
feelings of introjected guilt and external compulsion
(Sheldon
& Houser-Marko, 2001)—should be happier and more
satisfied with their goals, themselves, and ultimately
their lives. Our results link these two literatures by
showing that goals are one of the means by which
positivity has its effects.
Turning to the specific results,
Study 1 was conducted with students, as was the case
with most previous self-concordance studies. Here,
self-concordance was longitudinally associated with life
satisfaction. Previous self-concordance research (e.g.,
Koestner
et al., 2002; Sheldon
& Elliot, 1999) has typically used mood-focused
affect items, whereas this study used direct questions
about life satisfaction. This study shows that affective
implications of self-concordance are not confined to a
single affect scale. More important, Study 2 shows that
self-concordance in work goals leads to satisfaction at
work, much in the same way that self-concordance with
general life goals leads to life satisfaction in
previous research.
Specifically, the correlations
(see –2)
reveal that whereas the more autonomously chosen goals
typically had positive and significant correlations with
goal attainment, job satisfaction, and life
satisfaction, the “controlled” (introjected and
extrinsic) reasons typically had nonsignificant
correlations with these variables. It is worth asking
why goals chosen for controlled reasons had so little
effect on goal success and satisfaction. Consider, for
example, introjected goals, which amount to pursuing
goals because one would feel guilty by not pursuing
them. Such goals could actually be rational,
self-enhancing actions (e.g., eating healthy food,
exercising, being honest), but actions about which one
feels conflict. Thus, the beneficial effects of these
goals could be cancelled out by the fact that one was
not fully and personally committed to such goals.
Similarly, external goals “assigned” by others might
also be important goals, but less satisfying because
they are pursued for others, or because one feels quiet
resistance to them.
Why do people adopt controlled
goals at all if they produce such difficulties in
self-regulation? Sheldon
and Elliot (1998) argued that such goals are
selected when individuals fail to assess accurately
their inner needs, values, and interests. As such,
people who lack this type of inner self-information may
select goals based on perceived external requirements or
may incorporate the needs, demands, or values of others
as one's own (introjection) rather than the needs of the
self. However, because goals derived from controlled
processes do not represent real interests of the self,
they may unintentionally fade, despite the individuals'
best original intentions to keep at them. As a result,
individuals who pursue controlled goals may be unlikely
to attain these goals or may find their attainment less
pleasurable, either of which may lead to
dissatisfaction.
One of the more important
contributions of this research was to illuminate the
effect of core self-evaluations on self-concordance and
its consequences. In both studies, there were
significant associations between core self-evaluations
and self-concordance. People who see themselves as
worthy, efficacious, and in control of their lives
(positive core self-evaluations) were most likely to set
goals for autonomous or self-chosen reasons. According
to Sheldon
and Houser-Marko (2001), the ability to select
self-concordant goals is a difficult skill that requires
both “accurate self-perceptual abilities and the ability
to resist social pressures that may sometimes push one
in inappropriate directions” (p. 162). Our research
showed that the core self-evaluations concept is a
potentially important factor influencing this ability
for successful goal pursuit. In other words, people with
positive core self-evaluations were especially good in
demonstrating this adaptable ability to select
“self-concordant” goals that represent their implicit
interests, values, and growth needs, and were more
satisfied with their jobs and lives in turn.
Somewhat surprisingly, the
results involving goal attainment were relatively weak.
Perceived goal attainment failed to mediate the
relationship between self-concordance and satisfaction.
Furthermore, the relationship between self-concordance
and goal attainment, while significant in both studies,
was not strong (see Figures
3 and 5).
Though little research has linked goals and job
satisfaction (for an exception with organizational
newcomers, see Maier
& Brunstein, 2001), the weak association between
goal progress or attainment and job satisfaction, under
the auspices of the self-concordance model, is contrary
to past self-concordance research. The relationship
between goals and job satisfaction is complex, perhaps
more complex than we have been able to model in the
present study. Whereas setting difficult performance
goals for oneself may be dissatisfying in the short term
because they may cause the individual to have low
expectations for goal attainment (Mento,
Locke, & Klein, 1992), the attainment of those
goals (which is facilitated by the setting of difficult
goals) is argued to lead to satisfaction (Locke
& Latham, 1990). Because we did not separate
goal expectancy from goal attainment in our study,
further study of this relationship is warranted.
Although we found in our study
some support to that effect in the direct relationship
between core self-evaluations and satisfaction, we also
found some support for an indirect effect through
dynamic processes in the form of goal pursuit. Our
results join an increasing body of research (e.g., Koestner
et al., 2002; Sheldon
& Houser-Marko, 2001; Sheldon et al., 2002) that
shows that it is possible to become more satisfied with
job and life through one's pursuits, if one picks the
right goals and does well with them. In fact, our
results may suggest that core self-evaluations may serve
more like a trigger than an anchor. People with positive
core self-evaluations strive “for the right reasons,”
and therefore “get the right results,” both of which in
turn increase their levels of satisfaction. Moreover,
such increases in levels of satisfaction appear to last
(both of our studies were longitudinal) and perhaps lead
to even more positive changes in an upward spiral of
positive outcomes (Ryff
& Singer, 1998). For individuals with less
positive core self-evaluations, it is particularly
important that such individuals introspect on the nature
of their goals and the reasons they pursue them because
there is a greater risk that they pursue goals for
controlled reasons. Such introspection may hold the
promise of leading to an alteration or reappraisal of
goals and goal pursuit, which could then initiate the
aforementioned upward spiral.
There are several limitations to
our studies. First, the reliabilities of some of the
individual scales were low. For example, the Intrinsic
and Identified Scales in Study 1, the measure of goal
attainment in Study 1, and the Locus of Control Scale in
Study 2 were below.70. However, with the exception of
the goal attainment measure in Study 1 (α =.68), these
scales were used as indicators of the broader concepts
that were included in the causal model. Specifically,
measures of the motives underlying goal pursuit and
measures of locus of control were used as indicators of
an overall self-concordance and core self-evaluations
factor, respectively. These overall factors were
measured reliably across all three studies (¯α =.80).
Thus, that the individual scales were, in some cases,
measured unreliably does not mean that the factors the
scales indicated were measured unreliably. The marginal
reliability of goal attainment measure in Study 1,
however, should be acknowledged.
Second, the variables were
collected from the same source. However, because all
studies were longitudinal, temporal separation is one
means of reducing same-source bias (Podsakoff,
MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003). A third
limitation is that the relationships between goal
attainment and satisfaction, though significant, were
not very high (
=.20 in Study 1 and
=.16 in Study 2). In a job setting, satisfaction is not
just a function of the attainment of goals over which
one has control. It is also a function of job conditions
(e.g., leadership, coworkers, working conditions, and
compensation) for which goal setting may be unrealistic.
Even in a student setting, life satisfaction may be
affected by events and conditions over which one has
limited influence (e.g., looks, wealth, family
relationships, and faith). Future studies of job
satisfaction would do well to include measures of
satisfaction with the personal job strivings divorced
from uncontrollable conditions, although it is notable
that the correlation with overall job satisfaction was
significant nonetheless.
Finally, though past
self-concordance research has used an overall
self-concordance composite (Koestner
et al., 2002; Sheldon
& Elliot, 1999; Sheldon
& Houser-Marko, 2001), it may be productive for
future research to investigate the motives separately.
As noted earlier, we did find differential validity by
the four self-concordance components, with intrinsic
motives generally being the most strongly related to
satisfaction. Because the intrinsic and extrinsic
dimensions anchor the extremes of the self-concordance
continuum (Sheldon
& Houser-Marko, 2001), these two dimensions may
be the most appropriate to consider in future
research.
Despite these limitations, the
study makes some significant contributions to the
literature on antecedents of job and life satisfaction.
First, this study showed that it is possible not only to
become happier through one's goal pursuit, as other
studies have showed (see Sheldon
& Houser-Marko, 2001), but also that it is
possible to become more satisfied with one's job through
choosing the right goals. Second, our study showed that
people who are more self-positive (high core
self-evaluations) tend to choose the goals that have the
best chance to make them happy (with their jobs and
lives). Third, we found that the pursuit of
self-concordant goals may explain part of the
relationship between dispositions and the feelings of
fulfillment and satisfaction. Finally, the incorporation
of goals in the job satisfaction literature has another
distinct advantage. Although goal constructs such as
self-concordance show enough temporal stability to
affect variables of interest over time, self-concordant
beliefs have shown an ability to change when individuals
receive instruction on how to do so (Sheldon et al.,
2002). Precisely because of their flexibility and
sensitivity to changing contexts, it is possible to help
people pursue “better goals.” The investigation of such
interventions may be a fruitful target for future
research, a research that may help people in identifying
ways to improve their jobs and their lives. References
Best, R. G. (2003). Are
self-evaluations at the core of job burnout?
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Kansas State
University.
Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A.
(2003). Self-concordance at work: Toward understanding
the motivational effects of transformational leaders.
Academy of Management Journal, 46,
554–571.
Brayfield, A. H., & Rothe,
H. F. (1951). An index of job satisfaction. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 35, 307–311.
Brockner, J. (1988).
Self-esteem at work: Research, theory, and
practice. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae,
R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory
(NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor (NEO-FFI) Inventory
professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M.
(1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination
in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
Diener, E. (2000). Subjective
well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for
a national index. American Psychologist,
55, 34–43.
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A.,
Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The
Satisfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 49, 71–75.
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas,
R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being:
Three decades of progress. Psychological
Bulletin, 125, 276–302.
Eisenberg, A. P. (2000). The
search for integrity: A leadership impact study.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, DePaul
University.
Elliot, A. J., & Sheldon, K.
M. (1997). Avoidance achievement motivation: A personal
goals analysis. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 73, 171–185.
Elliot, A. J., & Sheldon, K.
M. (1998). Avoidance personal goals and the
personality-illness relationships. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 75,
1282–1299.
Elliot, A. J., Sheldon, K. M.,
& Church, M. A. (1997). Avoidance personal goals and
subjective well-being. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 23, 915–927.
Emmons, R. A. (1992). Abstract
versus concrete goals: Personal striving level, physical
illness, and psychological well-being. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 62,
292–300.
Emmons, R. A., & Diener, E.
(1986). A goal-affect analysis of everyday situational
choices. Journal of Research in Personality,
20, 309–326.
Erez, A., & Judge, T. A.
(2001). Relationship of core self-evaluations to goal
setting, motivation, and performance. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 86, 1270–1279.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck,
S. B. G. (1968). Manual for the Eysenck Personality
Inventory. San Diego, CA: EDITS..
Harter, S. (1990). Causes,
correlates, and the functional role of global
self-worth: A life-span perspective. In R. J. Sternberg
& J. Kolligan, Jr. (Eds.), Competence
considered (pp. 67–97). New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F.
L. (1990). Methods of meta-analysis. Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom,
D. (1993). LISREL 8: Structural equation modeling
with the SIMPLIS command language. Chicago, IL:
Scientific Software International.
Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E.
(2001). Relationship of core self-evaluations
traits—self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of
control, and emotional stability—with job satisfaction
and job performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 86, 80–92.
Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Erez,
A., Locke, E. A., & Thoresen, C. J. (2002). The
scientific merit of valid measures of general concepts:
Personality research and core self-evaluations. In
J.Brett & F.Drasgow (Eds.), The psychology of
work: Theoretically based empirical research (pp.
55–77). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., &
Locke, E. A. (2000). Personality and job satisfaction:
The mediating role of job characteristics. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 85, 237–249.
Judge, T. A., Erez, A., Bono, J.
E., & Thoresen, C. J. (2002). Are measures of
self-esteem, neuroticism, locus of control, and
generalized self-efficacy indicators of a common core
construct?Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 83, 693–710.
Judge, T. A., Erez, A., Bono, J.
E., & Thoresen, C. J. (2003). The Core
Self-Evaluations Scale (CSES): Development of a measure.
Personnel Psychology, 56,
303–331.
Judge, T. A., & Hulin, C. L.
(1993). Job satisfaction as a reflection of disposition:
A multiple source causal analysis. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 56,
388–421.
Judge, T. A., & Larsen, R.
J. (2001). Dispositional source of job satisfaction: A
review and theoretical extension. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86,
67–98.
Judge, T. A., Locke, E. A.,
& Durham, C. C. (1997). The dispositional causes of
job satisfaction: A core evaluations approach.
Research in Organizational Behavior,
19, 151–188.
Judge, T. A., Locke, E. A.,
Durham, C. C., & Kluger, A. N. (1998). Dispositional
effects on job and life satisfaction: The role of core
evaluations. Journal of Applied Psychology,
83, 17–34.
Koestner, R., Lekes, N., Powers,
T. A., & Chicoine, E. (2002). Attaining personal
goals: Self-concordance plus implementation intentions
equals success. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 83, 231–244.
Levenson, H. (1981).
Differentiating among internality, powerful others, and
chance. In H. M.Lefcourt (Ed.), Research with the
locus of control construct (pp. 15–63). New York:
Academic Press.
Locke, E. A. (1991). The
motivation sequence, the motivation hub, and the
motivation core. Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 50, 288–299.
Locke, E. A. (1997). The
motivation to work: What we know. Advances in
Motivation and Achievement, 10,
375–412.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G.
P. (1990). Work motivation and satisfaction: Light at
the end of the tunnel. Psychological Science,
1, 240–246.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G.
P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal
setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey.
American Psychologist, 57,
705–717.
Locke, E. A., McClear, K., &
Knight, D. (1996). Self-esteem and work.
International Review of Industrial and
Organizational Psychology, 11, 1–32.
Maier, G. W., & Brunstein,
J. C. (2001). The role of personal work goals in
newcomers' job satisfaction and organizational
commitment: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 86, 1034–1042.
Mento, A. J., Locke, E. A.,
& Klein, H. J. (1992). Relationship of goal level to
valence and instrumentality. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 77, 395–405.
Peale, N. V. (1952). The
power of positive thinking. New York: Prentice
Hall.
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S.
B., Lee, J., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common
method biases in behavioral research: A critical review
of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 88, 879–903.
Roberson, L. (1990). Prediction
of job satisfaction from characteristics of personal
work goals. Journal of Organizational Behavior,
11, 29–41.
Rosenberg, M. (1965).
Society and the adolescent self-image.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Rotter, J. B. (1966).
Generalized expectancies for internal versus external
control of reinforcement. Psychological
Monographs, 80( 1, Whole No. 609).
Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B.
(1998). The contours of positive human health.
Psychological Inquiry, 9, 1–28.
Seligman, M. E. P., &
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An
introduction. American Psychologist,
55, 5–14.
Shah, J., & Higgins, E. T.
(2001). Regulatory concerns and appraisal efficiency:
The general impact of promotion and prevention.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
80, 693–705.
Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A.
J. (1998). Not all personal goals are personal:
Comparing autonomous and controlled reasons for goals as
predictors of effort and attainment. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 24,
546–557.
Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A.
J. (1999). Goal striving, need satisfaction, and
longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
76, 482–497.
Sheldon, K. M., &
Houser-Marko, L. (2001). Self-concordance, goal
attainment, and the pursuit of happiness: Can there be
an upward spiral?Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 80, 152–165.
Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T.
(1998). Pursuing personal goals: Skills enable progress,
but not all progress is beneficial. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 24,
1319–1331.
Tait, M., Padgett, M. Y., &
Baldwin, T. T. (1989). Job and life satisfaction: A
reevaluation of the strength of the relationship and
gender effects as a function of the date of the study.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 74,
502–507.
Watson, D. (2000). Mood and
temperament. New York: Guilford Press.
Submitted: June 4,
2003 Revised: January 28, 2004 Accepted:
February 9, 2004
Copyright 2005 American
Psychological Association This publication is
protected by US and international copyright laws
and its content may not be copied without the
copyright holders express written permission
except for the print or download capabilities of
the retrieval software used for access. This
content is intended solely for the use of the
individual
user.
Source: Journal
of Applied Psychology. Vol.90 (2) pp.
257-268. Accession
Number: apl-90-2-257 Digital
Object
Identifier: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.2.257 | |